This is a topic you will find that is personal to me. Initially, I chose to research the reliability of traumatic memory and with peer editing added in the variable of how it applies to the real world. The essay is proving two points simultaneously: memory is complex and there are few personnel that are truly qualified to properly assist a subject to extract traumatic memories while keeping in mind the subject's well-being.
The Complexity of Traumatic Memory
“Regardless of whether
one accepts the concept of traumatic amnesia … memory does not operate like a
video tape, emotionality does not confirm veracity, and implicit memory cannot
simply be translated into narrative memory” (Colin et. al.). When I was eight,
I was diagnosed with a chronic, incurable condition known as Hydrocephalus and
have had ten brain surgeries since to treat it. One of the worst years,
medically-speaking, was 2017 and I can only remember bits and pieces of it. It
was filled with trauma after trauma, and my brain protected me and is still
trying to protect me from fully experiencing the events of that year. As
someone with post-traumatic stress disorder, I can say that my memory is
tainted by pain felt, and is faulty in terms of the chronological order of
events and my perception. Thankfully, the accuracy of my memory has not been
scrutinized in a congressional hearing, but if it were, there would be strong
eye-witness evidence which would prove my testimony somewhat incomplete and
inaccurate. Although human memory is complex and varied, traumatic memory is less
reliable than general memory because the brain is not naturally able to
experience and recount trauma. Traumatic memory, as said before, is tainted by
extreme emotion while recalling when you put away dishes yesterday is easily
recalled with little to no emotion. When a criminal investigation is being
held, the criminal investigator/psychologist has only two agendas: to know what
happened and to build a profile (“Forensic
Psychology vs. Clinical Psychology: Choosing a Path.”). Neither of those
agendas will result in the most accurate recall because from experience, you
must feel and cope with the pain of a memory before knowing what exactly caused
it.
Everyone knows the
catchy phrase, “fight or flight”. It represents how one responds to a situation
that their brain deems as life or death. You can either fight the subject(s)
that you feel are a harm to you or you can run from them. But the phrase “fight
or flight” forgets to mention the “freeze” response to traumatic events. A
phrase one hears from sexual assault victims and other victims of trauma when
explaining what happened during it is, “I froze”, “I didn’t know what to do” (“What Happens to the
Brain During a Sexual Assault?”). This is the freeze response. The inaccuracy
of traumatic memory is mainly caused by protective mechanisms in the brain. One
natural tendency to avoid the “overwhelming traumatic experience” is called
“dissociation.” Dissociation in the brain works like circuits on a house. When
an event triggers a flood of overwhelming emotions, the brain shuts it all off.
This is not a physical escape; it’s a psychological escape that is a
“compartmentalization” in which “aspects of psychological function that should
be associated… are not” (Lanius par. 1). This separates the person’s mind and
spirit from the ongoing event and from experiencing the event to its’ full
extent. Recalling traumatic memory is complex due to the handful of protective
mechanisms the brain can choose from. Only a licensed clinical psychologist can
properly handle this due to their extensive knowledge and agenda of the health
of the patient in recalling trauma.
The heavier, or more traumatic, the
trauma, the more “likelihood that an individual will be driven into an altered
state of conscious” (Lanius par.2). This means, the more traumatic something is
to a person, the more they dissociate from it. Based on my personal experience, during and after a
trauma, my brain could be in survival mode, as known as dissociation, for a
period of time. This constant mental state of survival and reliving the trauma
can lead to a decrease in the mass of the hippocampus and amygdala; hence,
making memory recall more difficult and dissociation from the trauma more
likely (Toth and Cicchetti 592, 594-597). The hippocampus and amygdala are
parts of the brain known for controlling and regulating emotion and memory
(Hayes 1-4, 8-11). So, it would make sense those parts of the brain are being
affected. If those parts of the brain are decreasing in mass, they are becoming
deformed, which would directly cause memory problems. The brain will
protect itself to the extent of destroying itself and induce separation of
consciousness and awareness to attempt avoiding a constant state of being
“overruled” by emotions (Ehlers 142). What makes the brain dissociate from a
situation is the highly emotional state it forces the brain to go into. Since
the brain of a victim of trauma is trying to protect him/her from the event(s),
when having to recall those memories, the subject will lack important detail and
facts, chronological order, self-awareness and possess a skewed perception of
the memory due to the overwhelming emotions (Ehlers 141-143). Unlike traumatic
memories, general memories don’t send the brain into survival mode or affect
the subject’s emotional health and physical health, an indicator that general
memory isn’t processed the same as traumatic memory. Since protective
mechanisms can cause gaps in memory, a clinical psychologist can help with
memory recall techniques that they get education in during school. Doing this,
a subject will be more likely to recall a memory without the full emotional
charge.
General memory and
traumatic memory differ in terms of the easiness of processing and ability to
recall. Since traumatic memories are more difficult to recall and for the brain
to process and store, this would imply that general, mundane memories, are
easier for the brain to store, encode, and interpret (Ehlers 141-143). This
means that the brain does not easily store and process traumatic memory because
trauma itself is not an enjoyable experience. To support the point that trauma
memory is more difficult for the brain to accurately encode further, studies
conclude that “people tend to remember more trauma than they experienced”
better explained as “memory amplification” (Strange and Takarangi par. 2). For
example, if multiple people were to experience the same traumatic event
together, each person would have a memory of things that are significant to the
individual; hence, details important to each person will be amplified and
presented as an accurate memory. When a person is trying to recall a memory
that doesn’t have “the origins of each individual detail,” he uses
“heuristics”, our natural, instant thought about a certain event (Strange and
Takarangi par. 2). To put it another way, every time a subject recalls a
traumatic memory, their initial thought or emotion tied to the memory will
taint that memory. When he or she uses “heuristics” to recall a traumatic,
distant memory, those memories can be easily distorted and include details that
never happened and have a fake, yet believable “sense of familiarity” (Strange
and Takarangi par. 2). For example, when
I was enduring medical trauma last year, I projected that trauma on to my
relationships and all I could think is that those people hated me when in fact,
it was only the initial emotion that seemed to define the memory of the
relationships. Freud, although he had
some very controversial beliefs, also had a theory that coerces with this idea
and makes it even more complex. Memories are always being rearranged in our
mind every time we process it throughout our life. The brain rearranges our
memories throughout life based on more current experiences and decides what it
should revise (Kennedy 186). This is why something that didn’t seem traumatic
as a child could become traumatic as an adult. A lot of my childhood, as I look
back, was not a cookie-cutter little girl in the suburbs as I thought it was.
This realization and having to go back through it in therapy traumatized me for
a few years because once I aged and developed critical thinking, it then became
traumatic. More research done by
Sheree L. Toth and Dante Cicchetti suggests that subjects can’t accurately
encode memories due to the natural dissociative state of the brain when
experiencing trauma and recalling (reexperiencing) trauma (592). Their point is
that if the subject experienced trauma in a mental state where their brain has
shut off itself from the real world, how could the subject accurately recall
the event? Some researchers disagree and introduce an opposite, yet
evolutionary mechanism of the brain called “hypervigilance.” Even if this were
the case, then the traumatized subject would still need to be able to be in a
safe, nonobjective environment to explore their trauma.
For many years the fact that traumatic memory is
unreliable has been widely accepted by researchers, but recently, for a congressional hearing of Justice resulted in a sexual
assault accusation known as the “Kavanaugh Trial.” This incident brought the
reliability of traumatic memory into serious question (Chatterjee 142). In “
"How Trauma Affects Memory: Scientists Weigh In On The Kavanaugh
Hearing," Rhitu Chatterjee addresses the controversial trial regarding a
sexual assault accusation made by Christine Ford against Supreme Court nominee,
Brett Kavanaugh that occurred over 35 years ago. The controversy in this trial
is not the sexual assault itself but the fact that Mrs. Ford was able to
thoroughly recall and tell the events that happened on the night of her alleged
rape. Chatterjee explores this ability to recall traumatic events and uses
research performed by Jim Hopper at Harvard University, in which he finds that
“What we pay attention to is what is more likely to get encoded” (qtd. in
Chatterjee); In simpler terms, mundane, everyday tasks are more likely to be
forgotten rather than those that have “emotional significance” (Chatterjee).
Richard McNally at Harvard University also supports this conclusion with the
assumption that when a traumatic event happens ones “hippocampus [goes] into a
super-encoding mode… The central details [of the event] get burned into their
memory” (qtd. in Chatterjee) and are
“highly accessible” (Ehlers 143). Yet, with vivid, recountable memories, the
“peripheral details” are usually not encoded (Chatterjee). Additionally, it’s
important to note that evidence supporting the reliability of traumatic
memories due to hypervigilance is all but nonexistent pre-Kavanaugh’s congressional
hearing and in abundance
post-Kavanaugh. From personal experience, I can say that there are flashes of
images that I can vividly recount, but the memories themselves are out-of-order
and dramatized. My question for this hypervigilance theory is how does one’s
brain, during a traumatic event, pick what details are important or not? Well,
it’s usually what is threatening their well-being. An example given by Richard
Mcnally PhD and others, if someone were to get robbed at gun-point, they are
more like to remember the gun than the robber’s face (et al. 819). Of course
the brain would focus on the thing that is intended to kill them rather than
their robber’s face. Our brain will focus on sensory and high-risk objects so,
how could we expect to let memory to be scrutinized in a congressional
hearing based on what information a rushed investigator has managed to pull out
from a person’s memory. Getting a clinical psychologist to interact and extract
memories will be slower but more beneficial to the patient’s health and the
most accurate it could be.
When it comes to memory, general memory is easier to
accurately retrieve than traumatic memory due to the protective mechanism of
the brain, dissociation. The counter-arguments presented are supported by
mostly political-based research and are contradicted by many reliable
researchers stating that traumatic memory is unreliable. It is not my purpose
to question the trauma that happened to individuals but to bring awareness on
just how unreliable and difficult to navigate traumatic memory can be in
comparison to general memory. Based on the fragility of memory in the evidence
of this essay, the actions that should be taken, whether it’s in a court of law
or not, only a licensed expert should help the victim process their trauma and
help dilute the strong, unmanageable emotions that come with it. A court
psychologist will have a bias. The bias of the objective to figure out the
what, when and where of the event but they don’t have the education in
psychology as much as clinical psychologists. Clinical
psychologists must at least get a master’s degree and most end up getting a
PhD. While court psychologists only have to get a bachelor’s degree and are
able to go straight into the field (“Forensic
Psychology vs. Clinical Psychology: Choosing a Path.”). Two years is not enough
time to learn about the brain let alone, how traumatic experiences affect its
memory. In conclusion, there is
still much more research to be done in this subject but it can confidently be
said that traumatic memory is more difficult for the brain to process and
therefore, requires a professional who has studied for years and has no
objective other than for the patient to extract traumatic memories in a healthy
way.
Works Cited
Lanius, Ruth A. “Trauma-related
dissociation and altered states of consciousness: a call for clinical,
treatment, and neuroscience research” European
journal of psychotraumatology vol. 6 27905. 19 May. 2015,
doi:10.3402/ejpt.v6.27905. Accessed 4 March 2020.
Ehlers, Anke. “Understanding
and Treating Unwanted Trauma Memories in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder” Zeitschrift fur Psychologie vol. 218,
no. 2, 2010, pp. 141-145. Accessed
4 March 2020.
Chatterjee, Rhitu. "How Trauma Affects Memory:
Scientists Weigh In On The Kavanaugh Hearing". NPR, 2018,
https://www.npr.org/sections/health-shots/2018/09/28/652524372/how-trauma-affects-memory-scientists-weigh-in-on-the-kavanaugh-hearing.
Accessed 4 March 2020.
TOTH, SHEREE L., and
DANTE CICCHETTI . “Remembering, Forgetting, and the Effects of
Trauma on Memory: A
Developmental Psychopathology Perspective .” Development
and Psychopathology, vol. 10, no. 4, 1998, pp. 589–605. Accessed 4 March 2020.
Strange, Deryn and
Melanie Takarangi . “Memory distortion for Traumatic Events: The Role of Mental
Imagery” Frontiers in psychiatry vol.
6 27. 23 Feb. 2015, doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00027. Accessed 4 March 2020.
"memory, n."
OED Online, Oxford University Press,
July 2018, www.oed.com/view/Entry/116363. Accessed 4 March 2020.
"trauma, n."
OED Online, Oxford University Press,
July 2018, www.oed.com/view/Entry/205242. Accessed 4 March 2020.
"Memory |
Psychology". Encyclopedia Britannica,
2018, https://www.britannica.com/science/memory-psychology. Accessed 4 March 2020.
Hayes, Jasmeet Pannu
et al. “Reduced hippocampal and amygdala activity predicts memory distortions
for trauma reminders in combat-related PTSD” Journal of psychiatric research vol. 45,5 (2010): 660-9. Accessed 4 March 2020.
Cameron Colin,
Alexandra Heber, and Richard J. McNally. "Re: Troubles in Traumatology,
and Debunking Myths about Trauma and Memory/Reply: Troubles in Traumatology and
Debunking Myths about Trauma and Memory." Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 51, no. 6, 2006, pp. 402-3. ProQuest, https://search.proquest.com/docview/222804441?accountid=39906. Accessed 4 March 2020.
Kennedy, Roger. “Memory and the Unconscious.” Memory: Histories, Theories, Debates,
edited by Susannah Radstone and Bill Schwarz, Fordham University Press, NEW
YORK, 2010, pp. 179–197. JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/j.ctt1c999bq.16. Accessed 5 Mar.
2020.
N.A. “Forensic Psychology vs. Clinical Psychology:
Choosing a Path.” Maryville Online, Maryville University,
online.maryville.edu/vs/forensic-psychology-vs-clinical-psychology/.
N.A. “What Happens to the Brain During a Sexual
Assault?” Acasa.us, ACASA,
acasa.us/what-happens-to-the-brain-during-a-sexual-assault/.
Comments
Post a Comment